

How you say something can totally change the meaning of your words. (Kyryk Ivan/Shutterstock)
In a nutshell
• Scientists discovered that Heschl’s gyrus, not just the superior temporal gyrus as previously thought, plays a crucial role in processing speech melody. This brain region doesn’t just detect sound features but transforms pitch patterns into meaningful categories.
• When researchers tested a macaque monkey with the same audio material, its brain processed the acoustic features but couldn’t categorize pitch accents as meaningful units. This suggests our ability to extract meaning from speech melody is distinctly human and likely shaped by language experience.
• This discovery could lead to better treatments for speech and language disorders, improve AI speech recognition systems, and enhance our understanding of communication challenges faced by people with brain injuries affecting Heschl’s gyrus.
EVANSTON, Ill. — When we talk, the melody of our words carries meaning beyond the words themselves. That rising tone at the end of questions, the stress on important words, or the dip in pitch when mentioning something familiar. These musical elements of speech are called prosody, and they’re essential to how we communicate.
Recent research published in Nature Communications has uncovered surprising details about how our brains process these speech patterns. Scientists from various American universities have found that a brain region called Heschl’s gyrus (HG) plays a key role in identifying and categorizing pitch patterns in speech. This discovery challenges what researchers previously thought; that the superior temporal gyrus (STG) was the main area for processing all speech aspects, including melody.
“The results redefine our understanding of the architecture of speech perception,” says study author Bharath Chandrasekaran, professor at Northwestern University, in a statement. “We’ve spent a few decades researching the nuances of how speech is abstracted in the brain, but this is the first study to investigate how subtle variations in pitch that also communicate meaning is processed in the brain.”
What Are Pitch Accents and Why Do They Matter?
The study specifically focuses on pitch accents, the rises and falls in pitch that highlight words and reveal a speaker’s intentions. The same sentence can mean completely different things depending on which words get emphasis through pitch changes. A rising pitch on a word suggests new or important information. A low dipping pitch implies the information is already known. Our brains must quickly decode these subtle pitch variations to fully grasp meaning and intent.
Inside the Brain: A Rare Research Opportunity


To see this process in action, the research team recorded brain activity from inside the human brain. They placed electrodes deep within the brains of 11 participants undergoing monitoring for epilepsy surgery.
“Typically, communication and linguistics research rely on non-invasive recordings from the surface of the skin, which makes it accessible but not very precise,” explains Dr. Taylor Abel, chief of pediatric neurosurgery at the University of Pittsburgh School of Medicine. “A collaboration between neurosurgeon-scientists and neuroscientists, like ours, allowed us to collect high-quality recordings of brain activity that would not have been possible otherwise, and learn about the mechanisms of brain processing in a completely new way.”
Participants listened to “Alice’s Adventures in Wonderland” while researchers tracked their brain responses to different pitch patterns. The results revealed something unexpected. While both brain regions responded to speech, Heschl’s gyrus was much better at distinguishing between different types of pitch accents.
“Our study challenges the long-standing assumptions how and where the brain picks up on the natural melody in speech — those subtle pitch changes that help convey meaning and intent,” says Gnanateja. “Even though these pitch patterns vary each time we speak, our brains create stable representations to understand them.”
More surprisingly, Heschl’s gyrus doesn’t just process the sound features of pitch but treats them as meaningful categories, similar to how we understand words as concepts rather than just sounds. Gnanateja mentions that this layer of meaning from speech melody is processed earlier in the brain than scientists previously thought.
What Makes Human Speech Processing Unique


To confirm their findings, the researchers also recorded brain activity in a macaque monkey listening to the same story. Unlike humans, monkeys don’t use or understand pitch accents, though they can process basic sound features.
As expected, the monkey’s brain responded to features like pitch and intensity but didn’t categorize pitch accents in the meaningful way human brains did. This comparison shows that processing pitch accents as meaningful linguistic categories seems to be uniquely human, likely developed through our experience with language.
The study also found that speech aspects are processed in different regions of the brain. The superior temporal gyrus primarily processes consonants and vowels, whereas pitch accents showed stronger representation in Heschl’s gyrus. This suggests our brains use specialized pathways for different aspects of speech.
Real-World Applications of This Research
These findings have important implications for understanding speech problems and language disorders. This research might lead to new treatments for conditions, including autism, speech problems after stroke, and language-based learning differences. It could help people who struggle to interpret or produce appropriate speech melodies.


“Our findings could transform speech rehabilitation, AI-powered voice assistants, and our understanding of what makes human communication unique,” says Chandrasekaran.
These discoveries might also improve AI systems by enhancing how they handle the musical aspects of speech, making computer speech recognition more human-like.
The Symphony in Our Speech
The brain machinery involved in processing speech melody shows how remarkably our brains extract meaning from sound. When we listen to someone, different brain regions are all working together to help us understand not just words but intentions and emotions.
This process happens automatically during conversations. Next time you recognize a question from someone’s tone before they finish speaking, or catch subtle emphasis that changes a phrase’s meaning, your Heschl’s gyrus is helping you decode the melody in their words.
In our digital world, where text messages often lack these melodic cues, understanding how our brains process these elements reminds us why face-to-face conversation feels richer. The rises and falls in pitch that go with our words aren’t just decorative; they’re fundamental to how we connect with each other.
Paper Summary
Methodology
The researchers used a technique that involves surgically implanting electrodes deep in the brain to record neural activity with high precision. The study included 11 adolescent patients already being monitored for epilepsy treatment. Participants listened to “Alice’s Adventures in Wonderland” while researchers tracked their brain responses to different pitch patterns. Mathematical models helped separate responses to pitch accent categories from responses to basic sound features.
Results
Of 158 speech-responsive areas in the brain, 63 could distinguish between different pitch accent types. This ability was much stronger in Heschl’s gyrus than in the superior temporal gyrus. Heschl’s gyrus wasn’t just responding to sound features but was treating pitch accents as distinct categories—similar to how we recognize words as meaningful units rather than just sounds. When the same test was done with a macaque monkey, its brain processed the sound features but didn’t categorize pitch accents as humans do, suggesting this ability is uniquely human.
Limitations
This study, while groundbreaking, has several limitations. The 11 participants were all being treated for epilepsy, which might affect how applicable the findings are to others. The electrode placement was determined by medical needs rather than research goals. The study used only one story read by one person, missing the variety of speech patterns across different speakers and situations. The comparison with non-human primates was based on just one monkey. Finally, while the study found how the brain processes pitch accents, it didn’t explore how these processes interact with other aspects of language understanding.
Funding and Disclosures
The research was supported by NIH grant 5R01DC13315-11 for the project “Cortical contributions to frequency-following response generation and modulation,” with investigators Bharath Chandrasekaran, Taylor Abel, Srivatsun Sadagopan, and Tobias Teichert. Additional funding came from NIH grant R21DC019217-01A1 to Taylor Abel, and from the University of Wisconsin-Madison to G. Nike Gnanateja. The researchers declared no competing interests, and all protocols were approved by the University of Pittsburgh’s Institutional Review Board.
Publication Information
This study, “Cortical processing of discrete prosodic patterns in continuous speech,” was published in Nature Communications on March 3, 2025 (Volume 16, Article number 1947). The research team included G. Nike Gnanateja and Kyle Rupp as co-first authors, with Taylor J. Abel and Bharath Chandrasekaran as supervisors, along with Fernando Llanos, Jasmine Hect, James S. German, and Tobias Teichert from institutions including University of Wisconsin-Madison, University of Pittsburgh, The University of Texas at Austin, Aix-Marseille University, and Northwestern University.