00:19 GMT - Monday, 07 April, 2025

When Bees Get Stressed, They Become Pessimists

Home - Family & Relationships - When Bees Get Stressed, They Become Pessimists

Share Now:

Posted 5 days ago by inuno.ai


Bumblebee pollinating a flowerBumblebee pollinating a flower

Bumblebees’ behavior changes when under stressful situations. (ArtEvent ET/Shutterstock)

In a nutshell

  • Bees can experience emotion-like states. When stressed, bumblebees behave pessimistically in uncertain situations, suggesting they may have internal states that influence their decisions, much like emotions do in humans.
  • Stress changes how bees interpret the world. After simulated predator attacks, bees were less likely to expect a high reward when faced with ambiguous cues, even though their memory and motivation were unaffected.
  • This matters for conservation. Since stress affects how bees forage and make decisions, environmental stressors like habitat loss and pesticides could have deeper impacts on pollination than previously understood.

NEWCASTLE UPON TYNE, England — Bumblebees can get down in the dumps, too. Researchers from Newcastle University discovered that when physically stressed, bees begin to expect worse outcomes when faced with uncertainty, behaving in ways that mirror how humans respond when anxious or depressed.

This research, published in Proceedings of the Royal Society B, challenges our understanding of insect minds and raises fascinating questions about the evolution of emotions across different branches of the animal kingdom.

For years, scientists have debated whether non-human animals experience emotions. While most research has focused on mammals and other vertebrates, growing evidence indicates insects may have more complex inner lives than we previously thought.

How Do You Measure a Bee’s Mood?

The research team wanted to test this in bumblebees but how do you measure what a bee is “thinking”? They couldn’t simply ask the bees about their emotional state. Instead, they developed a clever experiment to observe how stress changed the bees’ behavior when faced with ambiguous situations.

This approach builds on the concept of “judgment bias,” the way emotions color our interpretation of neutral or ambiguous information. People experiencing anxiety or depression tend to expect the worst from uncertain situations.

Previous studies with insects used what scientists call a “go/no-go” test design. But this approach has problems. If a stressed bee doesn’t approach something, is it because of a negative outlook, or simply because stress reduced its motivation? To overcome this limitation, the researchers designed a test requiring bees to make an active choice between two alternatives, making their judgment clearer.

Training Bees to Make Choices

A bird eating a bumblebeeA bird eating a bumblebee
Common predators of bumblebees are birds. (MyImages – Micha/Shutterstock)

In the experiment, female worker bumblebees learned to associate different colors with different quality rewards. When shown one color (like blue), they found a high-concentration sugar solution (50% sucrose) in one location. When shown another color (like green), they found a lower-concentration reward (30% sucrose) in a different location.

After this training, some bees were subjected to simulated predator attacks. One group was shaken (placed in a container and vibrated on a laboratory shaker for a minute), another group was briefly trapped by a mechanical device (simulating being caught by a predator), while a control group remained unstressed.

Then came the real test: researchers showed the bees not only the original trained colors but also new, ambiguous colors falling between the two training colors on the color spectrum. Both possible reward locations now contained only water. Which would the bees choose?

Stressed Bees Expect the Worst

Stressed bees were much less likely to approach the location that had previously offered higher rewards when shown ambiguous colors. They consistently made more “pessimistic” choices, behaving as if they expected lower rewards even though they could still distinguish between the original training colors accurately.

When researchers separately tested feeding motivation by measuring how much sugar solution different groups of bees consumed, they found no differences between stressed and non-stressed bees. This confirmed the behavioral change wasn’t simply due to reduced hunger after stress.

To better understand the bees’ decision-making process, the researchers applied mathematical modeling. Their analysis determined that stressed bees weren’t confused about the colors. Rather, they had shifted their expectations about rewards.

Control bees remained optimistic, behaving as if they expected high rewards to be available about half the time. But stressed bees acted as if high rewards were much less likely. The models revealed they behaved as if high rewards might be available as little as 6% of the time.

“Our research suggests that, like other animals including humans, bees may experience emotion-like states when stressed, as demonstrated by a clear shift towards pessimism. When faced with ambiguity, stressed bees, much like someone seeing the glass as ‘half empty,’ are more likely to expect negative outcomes,” says study author Olga Procenko from Newcastle University, in a statement.

More Than Just Tiny Robots

A bee landing on a flowerA bee landing on a flower
Studying how stress impacts bees can help inform conservation research. (SANDARU-KGL/Shutterstock)

From an evolutionary perspective, this pessimistic shift makes adaptive sense. After a dangerous encounter with a predator, playing it safe and expecting less from uncertain situations could help an animal avoid further danger. When the environment seems threatening, lowering expectations and taking fewer risks might increase survival chances.

Despite how small insect brains are, bees may experience internal states similar to emotions that influence their perception and decision-making in complex ways. Bees are critical pollinators facing numerous environmental stressors from pesticides to habitat loss and climate change. Scientists say the research is important as it means stress can impact how bees approach flowers and pollinate plants, as well as their ability to access high-quality rewards.

The humble bumblebee reminds us that the gap between insects and humans may be narrower than we once believed. Just like humans, they tend to be more pessimistic when under stress.

Paper Summary

Methodology

The experiment used female worker bumblebees trained to associate different colors with different rewards in specific locations. Bees were divided into three groups: a control group, a “shaking” stress group, and a “trapping” stress group. After stress exposure, bees were tested with five different colors (two training colors plus three ambiguous colors) with both potential reward locations containing only water. Separate tests measured feeding motivation to ensure behavior changes weren’t due to reduced hunger after stress.

Results

Stressed bees were significantly less likely to choose the location associated with high rewards when presented with ambiguous colors. Both shaking and trapping produced this pessimistic bias. All bees remained accurate when shown the original training colors, confirming memory and color discrimination weren’t impaired. Mathematical modeling revealed that stressed bees behaved as if they expected high rewards to be much less likely, despite no differences in feeding motivation between stressed and non-stressed bees.

Limitations

The study used a relatively small sample (48 bees) and only tested female workers. The laboratory environment and artificial stress manipulations may not perfectly mirror natural predation stress. Different treatments kept bees away from their colony for varying times, potentially introducing additional variables. The mathematical model estimated implausibly low expectations of reward in stressed bees (6% probability), suggesting the model may not perfectly capture bee cognition.

Discussion and Takeaways

This research provides strong evidence for emotion-like states in insects by using an active choice test design that eliminated alternative explanations. The findings suggest that basic emotional processes may be more widespread in the animal kingdom than previously thought. From an evolutionary perspective, pessimistic judgment biases after stress may be adaptive, helping animals avoid further risk when already vulnerable. Understanding how environmental stressors affect bee cognition has potential implications for conservation, especially given bees’ crucial role as pollinators.

Funding and Disclosures

The research was supported by a BBSRC David Phillips fellowship (BB/S009760/1) to Vivek Nityananda, and Olga Procenko was supported by a PhD scholarship from Newcastle University’s Faculty of Medical Sciences. The authors declared no competing interests.

Publication Information

The paper “Physically stressed bees expect less reward in an active choice judgement bias test” was published in Proceedings of the Royal Society B (2024). The authors are Olga Procenko, Jenny C.A. Read, and Vivek Nityananda from the Biosciences Institute at Newcastle University, UK.

Highlighted Articles

Subscribe
Notify of
0 Comments
Oldest
Newest Most Voted
Inline Feedbacks
View all comments

Stay Connected

Please enable JavaScript in your browser to complete this form.